The term “conspiracy theory” as a psycholinguistic tool for memetic hegemony

The conspiracy theory meme as a tool of cultural hegemony: A critical discourse analysis

by Rankin, James Edwin, Jr., Ph.D.

Abstract (Summary)

Those rejecting the official accounts of significant suspicious and impactful events are often labeled conspiracy theorists and the alternative explanations they propose are often referred to as conspiracy theories. These labels are often used to dismiss the beliefs of those individuals who question potentially hegemonic control of what people believe. The conspiracy theory concept functions as an impediment to legitimate discursive examination of conspiracy suspicions. The effect of the label appears to constrain even the most respected thinkers. This impediment is particularly problematic in academia, where thorough, objective analysis of information is critical to uncovering truth, and where members of the academy are typically considered among the most important of epistemic authorities. This dissertation tracked the development and use of such terms as pejoratives used to shut down critical thinking, analysis, and challenges to authority. This was accomplished using critical discourse analysis as a research methodology. Evidence suggesting government agents were instrumental in creating the pejorative meme conspiracy theorist was found in contemporary media. Tracing the evolution of the conspiracy theory meme and its use as a pejorative silencer may heighten awareness of its use in this manner and diminish its impact.

The term “conspiracy theory” was invented and put into public discourse by the CIA in 1964 in order to discredit the many skeptics who challenged the Warren Commission’s conclusion that President John F. Kennedy was assassinated by a lone gunman named Lee Harvey Oswald, who himself was assassinated while in police custody before he could be questioned. The CIA used its friends in the media to launch a campaign to make suspicion of the Warren Commission report a target of ridicule and hostility. This campaign was “one of the most successful propaganda initiatives of all time.”

This writes political science professor Lance deHaven-Smith, in his peer-reviewed book which was published by the University of Texas Press. He reports the story of how the CIA succeeded in creating in the public mind uncritical, reflexive, automatic, (System 1) stigmatization of those who challenge official government explanations (cf. ostracism).

Profile photo for

Lance DeHaven-Smith



According to Prof. DeHaven (see lecture above) the term “conspiracy theory” was first used in a scholarly book around 1913 by Charles Beard who used the phrase “the conspiracy theory of the 14th amendment”. DeHaven argues that if a wealthy women died because she fell in the shower and her husband inherits all her money we are automatically suspicious because of the low probability (base rate) of the incidence. If a similar situation happens again and the same husband is involved we are obviously even more suspicious. However, the term “conspiracy theory” prevents rational discourse (and rational thinking). DeHaven suggests the term “state crimes against democracy”. He makes the point that if we do not have a word for a crime it is very difficult to discuss it, especially if argumentators are discredited and ostracized as “conspiracy theorists” and categorized next to flat-earth believers (viz. invalid associations are created to facilitate superficial social categorisation).

Peer reviewed references on conspiracy theories – State crimes against democracy (multiple conspiracies are “organized crime”)

Related References

Elman, J. L.. (1999). Origins of language: A conspiracy theory. The Emergence of Language

Plain numerical DOI: 10.2514/3.19828
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Motter, G.. (2009). Conspiracy theory. EDN

Plain numerical DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.346500
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Miller, J. M., Saunders, K. L., & Farhart, C. E.. (2016). Conspiracy Endorsement as Motivated Reasoning: The Moderating Roles of Political Knowledge and Trust. American Journal of Political Science

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1111/ajps.12234
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Swami, V., Coles, R., Stieger, S., Pietschnig, J., Furnham, A., Rehim, S., & Voracek, M.. (2011). Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief system and associations between individual psychological differences and real-world and fictitious conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02004.x
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Darwin, H., Neave, N., & Holmes, J.. (2011). Belief in conspiracy theories. The role of paranormal belief, paranoid ideation and schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.027
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Swami, V.. (2012). Social psychological origins of conspiracy theories: The case of the Jewish conspiracy theory in Malaysia. Frontiers in Psychology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00280
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Wood, M. J., & Douglas, K. M.. (2013). What about building 7?” A social psychological study of online discussion of 9/11 conspiracy theories. Frontiers in Psychology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00409
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Newheiser, A. K., Farias, M., & Tausch, N.. (2011). The functional nature of conspiracy beliefs: Examining the underpinnings of belief in the Da Vinci Code conspiracy. Personality and Individual Differences

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.08.011
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Dagnall, N., Drinkwater, K., Parker, A., Denovan, A., & Parton, M.. (2015). Conspiracy theory and cognitive style: A worldview. Frontiers in Psychology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00206
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Xu, Z., Pothula, S. P., Wilson, J. S., & Apte, M. V.. (2014). Pancreatic cancer and its stroma: A conspiracy theory. World Journal of Gastroenterology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.3748/wjg.v20.i32.11216
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Geertz, C.. (1973). Thick Description: Toward an Interpretative Theory of Culture. In The Interpretation of Cultures

Plain numerical DOI: 10.4135/9781412984591.n6
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Van der Linden, S.. (2015). The conspiracy-effect: Exposure to conspiracy theories (about global warming) decreases pro-social behavior and science acceptance. Personality and Individual Differences

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.045
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Wood, M. J.. (2016). Some Dare Call It Conspiracy: Labeling Something a Conspiracy Theory Does Not Reduce Belief in It. Political Psychology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1111/pops.12285
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Pratt, R.. (2003). Theorizing conspiracy. Theory and Society

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1023/A:1023996501425
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Fekete, L.. (2012). The muslim conspiracy theory and the oslo massacre. Race and Class

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1177/0306396811425984
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Raab, M. H., Ortlieb, S. A., Auer, N., Guthmann, K., & Carbon, C. C.. (2013). Thirty shades of truth: Conspiracy theories as stories of individuation, not of pathological delusion. Frontiers in Psychology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00406
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Brotherton, R., French, C. C., & Pickering, A. D.. (2013). Measuring belief in conspiracy theories: The generic conspiracist beliefs scale. Frontiers in Psychology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00279
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Butt, L.. (2005). “Lipstick Girls” and “Fallen Women”: AIDS and Conspiratorial Thinking in Papua, Indonesia. Cultural Anthropology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1525/can.2005.20.3.412
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Briones, R., Nan, X., Madden, K., & Waks, L.. (2012). When Vaccines Go Viral: An Analysis of HPV Vaccine Coverage on YouTube. Health Communication

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1080/10410236.2011.610258
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Phillipson, R.. (2007). Linguistic imperialism: a conspiracy, or a conspiracy of silence?. Language Policy

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1007/s10993-007-9058-3
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Stojanov, A.. (2015). Reducing conspiracy theory beliefs. Psihologija

Plain numerical DOI: 10.2298/PSI1503251S
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Stempel, C., Hargrove, T., & Stempel, G. H.. (2007). Media use, social structure, and belief in 9/11 conspiracy theories. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1177/107769900708400210
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Craft, S., Ashley, S., & Maksl, A.. (2017). News media literacy and conspiracy theory endorsement. Communication and the Public

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1177/2057047317725539
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Gardener, T., & Moffat, J.. (2008). Changing behaviours in defence acquisition: A game theory approach. Journal of the Operational Research Society

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1057/palgrave.jors.2602476
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Sharp, D.. (2008). Advances in conspiracy theory. The Lancet

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61570-6
DOI URL
directSciHub download

The art of deception

The Art of Deception: Training for a New Generation of Online Covert Operations

theintercept.com/document/2014/02/24/art-deception-training-new-generation-online-covert-operations/
The Human Science Operations Cell (HSOC) is a division of the British signals intelligence agency, Government Communications Headquarters (GCHQ). The HSOC focus on “online human intelligence” and “strategic influence and disruption.”[1]

The existence of the HSOC was revealed as part of the global surveillance disclosures by the former National Security Agency contractor, Edward Snowden.[1] en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Human_Science_Operations_Cell

Fake news?

 

Lazer, D. M. J., Baum, M. A., Benkler, Y., Berinsky, A. J., Greenhill, K. M., Menczer, F., … Zittrain, J. L.. (2018). The science of fake news. Science

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1126/science.aao2998
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Conroy, N. J., Rubin, V. L., & Chen, Y.. (2015). Automatic deception detection: Methods for finding fake news. Proceedings of the Association for Information Science and Technology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1002/pra2.2015.145052010082
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Balmas, M.. (2014). When Fake News Becomes Real: Combined Exposure to Multiple News Sources and Political Attitudes of Inefficacy, Alienation, and Cynicism. Communication Research

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1177/0093650212453600
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Brigida, M., & Pratt, W. R.. (2017). Fake news. North American Journal of Economics and Finance

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1016/j.najef.2017.08.012
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Vargo, C. J., Guo, L., & Amazeen, M. A.. (2018). The agenda-setting power of fake news: A big data analysis of the online media landscape from 2014 to 2016. New Media and Society

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1177/1461444817712086
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Marchi, R.. (2012). With Facebook, blogs, and fake news, teens reject journalistic “objectivity”. Journal of Communication Inquiry

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1177/0196859912458700
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Rubin, V. L., Chen, Y., & Conroy, N. J.. (2015). Deception detection for news: Three types of fakes. Proceedings of the Association for Information Science and Technology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1002/pra2.2015.145052010083
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Frederiksen, L.. (2017). Fake News. Public Services Quarterly

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1080/15228959.2017.1301231
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G.. (2017). Who falls for fake news? The roles of bullshit receptivity, overclaiming, familiarity, and analytic thinking. SSRN

doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3023545

Balmas, M.. (2014). When Fake News Becomes Real. Communication Research

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1177/0093650212453600
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Wardle, C.. (2017). Fake news. It’s complicated. First Draft

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1038/nbt1002-973
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Bakir, V., & McStay, A.. (2018). Fake News and The Economy of Emotions: Problems, causes, solutions. Digital Journalism

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1080/21670811.2017.1345645
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Berghel, H.. (2017). Lies, Damn lies, and fake news. Computer

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1109/MC.2017.56
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Kucharski, A.. (2016). Post-truth: Study epidemiology of fake news. Nature

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1038/540525a
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Tandoc, E. C., Lim, Z. W., & Ling, R.. (2017). Defining “Fake News”. Digital Journalism

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1080/21670811.2017.1360143
DOI URL
directSciHub download

American psychological association & CIA: Science of Deception Workshop

APA Works with CIA and RAND to Hold Science of Deception Workshop

On July 17-18, RAND Corp. and the APA hosted a workshop entitled the “Science of Deception: Integration of Practice and Theory” with generous funding from the Central Intelligence Agency (CIA). The workshop provided an opportunity to bring together individuals with a need to understand and use deception in the service of national defense/security with those who investigate the phenomena and mechanisms of deception. Meeting at RAND headquarters in Arlington, VA, the workshop drew together approximately 40 individuals including research psychologists, psychiatrists, neurologists who study various aspects of deception and representatives from the CIA, FBI and Department of Defense with interests in intelligence operations. In addition, representatives from the White House Office of Science and Technology Policy and the Science and Technology Directorate of the Department of Homeland Security were present. Following brief introductions and welcoming remarks from Kevin O’Connell, Director of the Intelligence Policy Center within RAND’s National Security Research Division, workshop participants divided into break-out groups to discuss thematic scenarios following a format used in a previous conference on counterterrorism held at the FBI Academy in February 2002.

www.apa.org/about/gr/science/spin/2003/07/also-issue.aspx

Martin Luther King

The United States is “The Greatest Purveyor of Violence in the World Today” ~Martin Luther King

Frady, M.. (2003). Martin Luther King. Vita Breve
McGuire, D. M. G., & Hutchings, K. H.. (2007). Portrait of a transformational leader: The legacy of Dr Martin Luther King Jr. Leadership and Organization Development Journal

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1108/01437730710726840
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Stewart, C.. (2013). Martin Luther King, Jr. (1929-68). In The Student’s Companion to the Theologians

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1002/9781118427170.ch60
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Robinson, J. L., & Topping, D.. (2013). The Rhetoric of Power: A Comparison of Hitler and Martin Luther King Jr. Journal of Management Inquiry

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1177/1056492612451789
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Nelson Mandela

If there is a country that has committed unspeakable atrocities in the world, it is the United States of America. ~Nelson Mandela

Mandela, N.. (1994). The Long Walk to Freedom. In The Long Walk to Freedom

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1016/j.sysconle.2008.10.004
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Press, O. U.. (2013). Nelson Mandela: A Very Short Introduction. Journal of Chemical Information and Modeling

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1017/CBO9781107415324.004
DOI URL
directSciHub download

House, R. J., & Howell, J. M.. (1992). Personality and charismatic leadership. The Leadership Quarterly

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1016/1048-9843(92)90028-E
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Oppenheim, C. E.. (2012). Nelson Mandela and the Power of Ubuntu. Religions

Plain numerical DOI: 10.3390/rel3020369
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Lieberfeld, D.. (2003). Nelson Mandela: Partisan and peacemaker. NEGOTIATION JOURNAL-ON THE PROCESS OF DISPUTE SETTLEMENT

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1023/A:1024629628402
DOI URL
directSciHub download

O’Fallon, S.. (2012). Nelson Mandela and unitive eadership. Integral Leadership Review

Invisible facts and perceptual illusions – David Copperfield vanishing the Statue of Liberty

Large facts can be made invisible

de Gardelle, V., Sackur, J., & Kouider, S.. (2009). Perceptual illusions in brief visual presentations. Consciousness and Cognition

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.03.002
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Jazayeri, M., & Movshon, J. A.. (2007). A new perceptual illusion reveals mechanisms of sensory decoding. Nature

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1038/nature05739
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Smeets, J. B. J., Brenner, E., De Grave, D. D. J., & Cuijpers, R. H.. (2002). Illusions in action: Consequences of inconsistent processing of spatial attributes. Experimental Brain Research

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1007/s00221-002-1185-7
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Framing

In the social sciences, framing comprises a set of concepts and theoretical perspectives on how individuals, groups, and societies, organize, perceive, and communicate about reality. Framing involves social construction of a social phenomenon – by mass media sources, political or social movements, political leaders, or other actors and organizations. Recent findings in the domain of embodied cognition and conceptual metaphor theory are important theoretical frameworks in this context.

Benford, R. D., & Snow, D. A.. (2000). Framing Processes and Social Movements: An Overview and Assessment. Annual Review of Sociology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1146/annurev.soc.26.1.611
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N.. (2007). Framing Theory. Annual Review of Political Science

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1002/sia.3423
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Scheufele, D. A.. (1999). Framing as a theory of media effects. Journal of Communication

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.1999.tb02784.x
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Entman, R. M.. (1993). Framing: Toward Clarification of a Fractured Paradigm. Journal of Communication

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.1993.tb01304.x
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D.. (1986). Rational Choice and the Framing of Decisions. The Journal of Business

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1086/296365
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D.. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1126/science.7455683
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., & Gaeth, G. J.. (1998). All Frames Are Not Created Equal: A Typology and Critical Analysis of Framing Effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1006/obhd.1998.2804
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Scheufele, D. A., & Tewksbury, D.. (2007). Framing, agenda setting, and priming: The evolution of three media effects models. Journal of Communication

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1111/j.0021-9916.2007.00326.x
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Levin, I. P., Gaeth, G. J., Schreiber, J., & Lauriola, M.. (2002). A new look at framing effects: Distribution of effect sizes, individual differences, and independence of types of effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1006/obhd.2001.2983
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Kühberger, A.. (1998). The Influence of Framing on Risky Decisions: A Meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1006/obhd.1998.2781
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Snow, D. A., & Benford, R. D.. (2002). Ideology, Frame Resonance, and Participant Mobilization. International Social Movement Research

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1007/s00425-012-1590-5
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Halsnæs, K., Shukla, P., Ahuja, D., Akumu, G., Beale, R., Edmonds, J., … J. Zou. (2007). Framing issues. In Climate Change 2007: Mitigation. Contribution of Working Group III to the Fourth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1007/s10728-007-0063-7
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Nisbet, M., & Mooney, C.. (2007). Framing science. Science

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1002/cncy.20155
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Nelson, T. E., Oxley, Z. M., & Clawson, R. A.. (1997). Toward a psychology of framing effects. Political Behavior

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1023/A:1024834831093
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Pan, Z., & Kosicki, G. M.. (1993). Framing analysis: An approach to news discourse. Political Communication

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1080/10584609.1993.9962963
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Hallahan, K.. (1999). Seven Models of Framing: Implications for Public Relations. Journal of Public Relations Research

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1207/s1532754xjprr1103_02
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Ebbinghaus illusion

he Ebbinghaus illusion or Titchener circles is an optical illusion of relative size perception. Named for its discoverer, the German psychologist Hermann Ebbinghaus (1850–1909), the illusion was popularized in the English-speaking world by Edward B. Titchener in a 1901 textbook of experimental psychology, hence its alternative name.[1] In the best-known version of the illusion, two circles of identical size are placed near to each other, and one is surrounded by large circles while the other is surrounded by small circles. As a result of the juxtaposition of circles, the central circle surrounded by large circles appears smaller than the central circle surrounded by small circles.

Recent work suggests that two other critical factors involved in the perception of the Ebbinghaus illusion are the distance of the surrounding circles from the central circle and the completeness of the annulus, which makes the illusion comparable in nature to the Delboeuf illusion. Regardless of relative size, if the surrounding circles are closer to the central circle, the central circle appears larger and if the surrounding circles are far away, the central circle appears smaller. While the distance variable appears to be an active factor in the perception of relative size, the size of the surrounding circles limits how close they can be to the central circle, resulting in many studies confounding the two variables.[1]


Further References

Franz, V. H., Bülthoff, H. H., & Fahle, M.. (2003). Grasp effects of the Ebbinghaus illusion: Obstacle avoidance is not the explanation. Experimental Brain Research

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1007/s00221-002-1364-6
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Haffenden, A. M., Schiff, K. C., & Goodale, M. A.. (2001). The dissociation between perception and action in the Ebbinghaus illusion: Nonillusory effects of pictorial cues on grasp. Current Biology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00023-9
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Franz, V. H., & Gegenfurtner, K. R.. (2008). Grasping visual illusions: Consistent data and no dissociation. In Cognitive Neuropsychology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1080/02643290701862449
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Franz, V. H., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Bülthoff, H. H., & Fahle, M.. (2000). Grasping visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Psychological Science

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.00209
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Song, C., Schwarzkopf, D. S., & Rees, G.. (2011). Interocular induction of illusory size perception. BMC Neuroscience

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1186/1471-2202-12-27
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Doherty, M. J., Campbell, N. M., Tsuji, H., & Phillips, W. A.. (2010). The Ebbinghaus illusion deceives adults but not young children. Developmental Science

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00931.x
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Rose, D., & Bressan, P.. (2002). Going round in circles: Shape effects in the Ebbinghaus illusion. Spatial Vision

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1163/15685680252875165
DOI URL
directSciHub download

de Fockert, J., Davidoff, J., Fagot, J., Parron, C., & Goldstein, J.. (2007). More Accurate Size Contrast Judgments in the Ebbinghaus Illusion by a Remote Culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.3.738
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Metin, B., & Tavil, A.. (2014). Environmental assessment of external wall cladding construction. Architectural Science Review

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1080/00038628.2013.862610
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Parron, C., & Fagot, J.. (2007). Comparison of Grouping Abilities in Humans (Homo sapiens) and Baboons (Papio papio) With the Ebbinghaus Illusion. Journal of Comparative Psychology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1037/0735-7036.121.4.405
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Franz, V. H.. (2003). Planning versus online control: Dynamic illusion effects in grasping?. Spatial Vision

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1163/156856803322467491
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Vishton, P. M., Stephens, N. J., Nelson, L. A., Morra, S. E., Brunick, K. L., & Stevens, J. A.. (2007). Planning to reach for an object changes how the reacher perceives it. Psychological Science

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01965.x
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Plodowski, A., & Jackson, S. R.. (2001). Vision: Getting to grips with the Ebbinghaus illusion. Current Biology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00170-1
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Kopiske, K. K., Bruno, N., Hesse, C., Schenk, T., & Franz, V. H.. (2016). The functional subdivision of the visual brain: Is there a real illusion effect on action? A multi-lab replication study. Cortex

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.03.020
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Nakamura, N., Watanabe, S., & Fujita, K.. (2008). Pigeons perceive the Ebbinghaus-Titchener circles as an assimilation illusion.. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.34.3.375
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Danckert, J. A., Sharif, N., Haffenden, A. M., Schiff, K. C., & Goodale, M. A.. (2002). A temporal analysis of grasping in the Ebbinghaus illusion: Planning versus online control. Experimental Brain Research

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1007/s00221-002-1073-1
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Pickett, C. L.. (2001). The effects of entitativity beliefs on implicit comparisons between group members. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1177/0146167201275001
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Haffenden, A. M., & Goodale, M. A.. (1998). The effect of pictorial illusion on prehension and perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1162/089892998563824
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Sovrano, V. A., Albertazzi, L., & Rosa Salva, O.. (2014). The Ebbinghaus illusion in a fish (Xenotoca eiseni). Animal Cognition

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1007/s10071-014-0821-5
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Glover, S., & Dixon, P.. (2002). Dynamic effects of the Ebbinghaus illusion in grasping: Support for a planning/control model of action. Perception and Psychophysics

Plain numerical DOI: 10.3758/BF03195791
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Káldy, Z., & Kovács, I.. (2003). Visual context integration is not fully developed in 4-year-old children. Perception

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1068/p3473
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Byosiere, S. E., Feng, L. C., Woodhead, J. K., Rutter, N. J., Chouinard, P. A., Howell, T. J., & Bennett, P. C.. (2017). Visual perception in domestic dogs: susceptibility to the Ebbinghaus–Titchener and Delboeuf illusions. Animal Cognition

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1007/s10071-016-1067-1
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Duemmler, T., Franz, V. H., Jovanovic, B., & Schwarzer, G.. (2008). Effects of the Ebbinghaus illusion on children’s perception and grasping. Experimental Brain Research

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1007/s00221-007-1229-0
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Massaro, D. W., & Anderson, N. H.. (1971). Judgmental model of the Ebbinghaus illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1037/h0031158
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Roberts, B., Harris, M. G., & Yates, T. A.. (2005). The roles of inducer size and distance in the Ebbinghaus illusion (Titchener circles). Perception

Plain numerical DOI: 10.1068/p5273
DOI URL
directSciHub download

Lumen naturale

The natural ability to understand – to shine light on things – a natural light within.
“Lumen naturale” is a Latin term that translates to “natural light.” In the context of the Enlightenment, philosophers often used this concept to represent reason and knowledge that comes from nature and human intellect rather than relying solely on religious or traditional authorities.

Enlightenment thinkers believed in the power of reason and rationality to discover truths about the world. They saw the human mind as capable of understanding and improving society through the application of logic and scientific inquiry. The term “lumen naturale” reflects the idea that there is an inherent, natural capacity for humans to gain knowledge and insight through reason, independent of divine revelation.

This concept played a significant role in shaping the intellectual landscape of the Enlightenment, emphasizing the importance of human agency and critical thinking in the pursuit of knowledge and progress.


Further References

Jacquette, D.. (1996). Descartes’ Lumen Naturale and the Cartesian Circle. Philosophy and Theology, 9(3), 273–320.

→ Alphanumerical DOI: 10.5840/philtheol199693/41DOI URLDownload via SciHubTranslationForwardFormatBibTeXCitation Network GraphScite AI

“The author argues that descartes is not trapped inside the cartesian circle. the essay rehearses descartes’ argument against the ‘evil demon’ hypothesis. the so-called cartesian circle is described and some of the most prominent discussions of the problem are evaluated. such arguments tend either to leave descartes in the circle, or themselves depend upon distinctions that in the end lead to descartes claiming something less than metaphysical certainty for his system. the author argues that descartes’ real archimedian point is the light of nature, and that his project is to extend the certainty of the light of nature to those ideas which are clear and distinct. using this interpretation of descartes, the author returns to the accounts of the critics to account for their mischaracterization of descartes’ reasoning as circular.”
Apel, M.. (1950). Lagarde, Paul Anton de– lumen naturale. In Philosophisches Wörterbuch (pp. 151–157). Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter

→ Alphanumerical DOI: 10.1515/9783111380001-012DOI URLDownload via SciHubForwardFormatBibTeXCitation Network GraphScite AI

Opahle, O.. (1952). Die Lehre vom „lumen naturale“ in ihrer Bedeutung für die Pädagogik. Vierteljahrsschrift Für Wissenschaftliche Pädagogik, 28(2), 115–124.

→ Alphanumerical DOI: 10.30965/25890581-02802005DOI URLDownload via SciHubForwardFormatBibTeXCitation Network GraphScite AI